Introduction
The concept of political participation has expanded over the years to include a range of activities beyond traditional forms of involvement, such as protesting and volunteering for social causes (Ruess et al., 2021; Theocharis & van Deth, 2018). Social media platforms like Facebook and Twitter (X) have become major, and in some cases dominant, tools for political participation. The rise of social media has further facilitated and amplified new forms of political participation (Jost et al., 2018; Kim, 2015; Oser et al., 2013; Vaccari & Valeriani, 2021). While social media has become a prevalent means of social interaction due to its time- and cost-effectiveness (Matusitz, 2022), some researchers in the fields of democracy and participation have been skeptical or hesitant about the value of social media-based political action (Theocharis & Lowe, 2016; Vromen et al., 2016).
Participation, a form of communication (Hoffman, 2012), is an intrinsically voluntary activity involving ‘a complex process that includes doing, talking, thinking, feeling, and belonging’ as well as ‘action, e.g., talking with someone, and connection, e.g., feeling that one takes part’ (Hrastinski, 2009, p. 79). Moreover, political participation encompasses all dimensions of social activity designed directly to influence government agencies and the policy process, or indirectly to impact civil society, or which attempt to alter systematic patterns of social behavior (Norris, 2002, p. 16). This includes activities such as political discussion, social media engagement, and information seeking (Kim & Hoewe, 2020; Woo & Kim, 2009). Given these characteristics of participation and political participation, we can consider online political participation (OPP), a newer form of political involvement, to include social media-based political actions within the broader concept of political participation (Theocharis & Lowe, 2016; Vromen et al., 2016).
OPP is a type of political participation with unique attributes compared to traditional, offline participation. Specifically, OPP differs from traditional offline participation in that it utilizes new digital channels (Koc-Michalska & Lilleker, 2017; Lilleker & Koc-Michalska, 2018; Oser et al., 2013). Many studies have been conducted on the technical aspects of OPP and the factors that predict and influence it across various fields and countries. Studies on OPP typically examine factors such as a person’s demographic or political traits influencing their level of online participation (Barrett & Brunton-Smith, 2017; Kitanova, 2020; Oser & Boulianne, 2020), the impact of using new devices on participation levels moderated by a person’s digital literacy (Büchi & Vogler, 2017; Yue et al., 2019), and the interaction effect of both these factors (Ashley et al., 2017; Guess & Munger, 2023; Park, 2018). These articles focus more on who participates more frequently rather than how participants engage more frequently. The question of who participates addresses between-participant differences, whereas how participants engage concerns within-participant dynamics. This distinction necessitates an alternative methodological approach and a refined strategy for controlling the endogeneity of time-varying variables. Unfortunately, these crucial concerns remain under-addressed in the existing body of literature. This study poses the research question, ‘How does the use of social media influence individuals’ levels of OPP?’ The problem is that social media use (SMU) is influenced not only by various personal characteristics such as political attention, political awareness, and political sensitivity but also by online participation, the dependent variable. By exploring this question, this study aims to examine the extent to which this relationship can be empirically established while accounting for potential endogeneity issues.
This study contributes to the existing body of research in several ways. First, it examines the role of SMU on OPP, focusing on contrasting theoretical expectations in the literature. While this study was not able to directly measure the affiliated network, we assumed that the effect might vary depending on the characteristics of the network to which the participant belongs and explored this aspect. In doing so, this study provides further insights into the dynamics of social media and OPP, contributing to the understanding of online political mobilization. Second, it employs instrumental variable estimation with a fixed-effect model in the analysis to address potential endogeneity issues. This approach is significant not only for its methodological implications but also for distinguishing this study’s research questions from previous research that focused on between-variation. In so doing, this study offers clues for understanding the heterogeneous empirical results. Lastly, compared to most previous studies that use one-year cross-sectional data, this study utilizes panel data to enhance the statistical evidence. The structure of this study is as follows: the next section reviews related literature and discusses the impact of SMU on OPP based on various theories. The third and fourth sections describe the research methodology and variables. The fifth section presents the analysis results, and the final section discusses the practical and theoretical implications and potential limitations of the study.
Related Literature
There is ongoing disagreement in the literature on OPP, with some researchers arguing that OPP reinforces (i.e., normalizes) existing patterns of offline political participation, while others emphasize that social media can facilitate (i.e., mobilize) political engagement. Theories put forward by the former group are called normalization theories, while those proposed by the latter are referred to as mobilization theories. Normalization theories focus on the existing network structure, which reflects the status quo. According to normalization theorists, while it may appear that SMU brings about social change, these changes are largely driven by offline political dynamics in traditional political processes, and the impact of SMU is limited (Gerl et al., 2018; Mejias, 2010). On the other hand, mobilization theories focus on the horizontal and decentralized networks that are created and reinforced through the use of social media. Mobilization theorists generally posit that as the communication network becomes more connected and participatory through SMU, the population using the platform gains increased access to information, more opportunities to participate in public discussion, and greater capacity for collective action (Lilleker & Koc-Michalska, 2018; Shirky, 2011). They also believe that widespread SMU will promote the sharing of opinions in the public sphere, gradually building a framework for citizen participation over time (Boulianne, 2018; Shirky, 2013). Consequently, one’s perceived impact of SMU on OPP may depend on his/her acceptance of certain assumptions regarding network properties.
In addition to the divide between mobilization and normalization theories previously mentioned, there are two other notable strands of research within the field of OPP. One strand focuses on identifying the characteristics of individuals who participate online and primarily employs the resources approach (Best & Krueger, 2005; Boulianne, 2018; Ruess et al., 2023; Smith, 2013; Vasilescu et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2018). This strand, which generally assumes that SMU leads to OPP and that the network structure is decentralized and distributed, examines differences between individuals (i.e., between variations). The other strand of research on OPP focuses on examining the ways in which individuals engage in political activity. In doing so, it refers to social learning mechanisms and information exchange theories to understand the impact of SMU on OPP (Velasquez, 2012; Yamamoto et al., 2020). This area of research examines changes within an individual over time (i.e., within variations), taking into consideration that traditional resources related to connection intensity within a network are less likely to change over time. The latter strand of research on OPP implicitly assumes the status quo and may be more suitable for measuring the impact of an individual’s SMU on OPP, regardless of income, gender, age, and educational attainment. However, there is a significant risk of endogeneity in studies that adopt this assumption.
Normalization theories posit that the political potential of social media is primarily utilized by individuals who are already politically active and engaged. In other words, these theories propose that social media usage for political purposes does not significantly enhance overall participation (Chadwick, 2006; Gerl et al., 2018). This is attributed to preexisting inequalities in material, social, and political resources that also lead to disparities in online participation. This phenomenon, referred to as the ‘digital divide,’ closely mirrors disparities in offline participation among socio-demographic groups (Castells, 2008; Hirzalla et al., 2011; O’Neil, 2017; Pariser, 2011; Strauß et al., 2020; van Dijk, 2020).
Research on the digital divide, sometimes used interchangeably with normalization, has broadly examined two aspects of SMU: intensity and forms (Hirzalla et al., 2011). Studies have shown that differences in motivational, material, and skills access contribute to three socio-demographic gaps in most developed and democratic countries regarding the intensity of SMU: a ‘socioeconomic gap,’ a ‘gender gap,’ and an ‘age gap’ (di Gennaro & Dutton, 2006; Quintelier & Vissers, 2008). In terms of forms of SMU, research has focused on how access differences create or maintain a ‘democratic divide’ between those who can and cannot use the Internet and social media for political purposes (Matthews, 2021; Mossberger et al., 2003; Norris, 2001). These trends have led normalization theorists to view social media as reinforcing the status quo (e.g., Dalton, 2017), and they generally suggest that OPP is primarily influenced by political and social resources or a reflection of offline political participation (Bonfadelli, 2002; Matthews, 2021; Oates et al., 2006; van Dijk, 2005, 2006). However, it is important to note that digital divides are caused by differences in resources between individuals or groups, and socio-demographics alone do not fully explain differences in SMU (Hirzalla et al., 2011).1
Mobilization theories emphasize how social media can facilitate politically-motivated activities, serving as a political forum where individuals can develop civic skills and acquire the knowledge necessary for political engagement (e.g., Kann et al., 2007; Theocharis & van Deth, 2017; Valenzuela, 2013). According to Hirzalla et al. (2011), mobilization claims involve four types of OPP. First, there are studies on ‘digital activism,’ which refers to confrontational, sometimes highly radical, forms of interaction between citizens and political or economic elites (e.g., Dahlberg & Siapera, 2007; George & Leidner, 2019). Second, studies examine democratic conduct in discussions on web forums and social networking sites, with some scholars considering whether social media is realizing a new iteration of the public sphere concept (e.g., Bennett & Pfetsch, 2018; Dahlgren, 2005). Third, a body of research examines how individuals utilize the informative or educational potential of websites with politically or civically oriented content. These studies often aim to demonstrate the wealth of information available online to the general public, and how social media grants new freedoms to non-governmental organizations (NGOs), governments, and individual political entrepreneurs to disseminate their messages (Feezell et al., 2016; Montgomery et al., 2004). Fourth, much research oriented towards mobilization focuses on the concept of ‘e-democracy,’ which refers to an interactive online environment where citizens can educate themselves about and communicate with political representatives (Aichholzer & Rose, 2020; Chadwick, 2006).
Informed by the existing literature, we can summarize that normalization theories focus on citizens’ general abilities and goals, while mobilization theories examine the potential and actual instances of political activity on social media (Hirzalla et al., 2011). Normalization theories concentrate on identifying who participates and the socio-demographic differences among participants. According to normalization theory, OPP is seen as one of the socio-demographic characteristics of an individual. Therefore, it does not significantly increase or decrease depending on SMU, especially in estimates using a model with fixed individual characteristics. In contrast, mobilization theories are more interested in how participants engage in the political forum, how their behavior changes over time, and the features and characteristics of networks that facilitate and enable OPP. According to mobilization theory, as the average individual’s use of social media increases, so does the level of OPP. While these two theories present antithetical expectations, which one has more explanatory power in the current situation has not yet been empirically tested.
Considering the debate on whether SMU leads to normalization (where online political engagement mirrors existing offline patterns) or mobilization (acting as a catalyst for increased and diverse political participation), our first hypothesis is:
If the use of social media has a mobilization effect, what becomes important are the consequences of this mobilization. While most studies assume that it leads to an increase in OPP, this outcome can depend on the characteristics of the network, such as homophily or heterophily.
Homophily, the tendency of individuals to form relationships with others similar to themselves, is based on individual traits such as age, gender, race, educational attainment, or personality, or on group membership or shared experiences (Bond & Sweitzer, 2022; Boutyline & Willer, 2016; Granovetter, 1983; McPherson et al., 2001). This tendency can lead to the formation of homophilous clusters within networks, where individuals with similar characteristics or interests are more likely to connect. It is widely accepted that homophilous clusters play a significant role in promoting political participation (Christakis & Fowler, 2009; Kim & Chen, 2016; McPherson et al., 2001).
Meanwhile, heterophily refers to the tendency of individuals to associate with those who are dissimilar, based on individual characteristics or group membership (Boutyline & Willer, 2016; Halberstam & Knight, 2016; McPherson et al., 2001). This concept is often discussed regarding whether interactions with different individuals within the network will reinforce existing consistencies or lead to more accurate perceptions through mutual understanding (e.g., Song et al., 2020). Communication with heterogeneous individuals does not always reduce the consistency of beliefs; at least, such reduction may lead to a decrease in partisan attitudes and, further, a decrease in actual participation in the context of mobilization theory. However, to the best of our knowledge, there is a lack of prior research on the types of networks formed by individuals on social media platforms and their potential impact on facilitating OPP.
In terms of the role of networks, the impact of heterophily, and by extension homophily, on political participation, including OPP, remains inconclusive according to Valenzuela et al. (2012). Some research has found that high levels of heterophily can lead to political ambivalence and fear of isolation, resulting in decreased participation (e.g., Chu & Yeo, 2020; Mutz, 2006), while other studies have indicated that heterophily is positively related to participation (e.g., Scheufele et al., 2006; Strauß et al., 2020). Therefore, it can be hypothesized that the formation of networks characterized by homophily through SMU may enhance OPP, whereas the absence of such networks may decrease OPP. Furthermore, if the remaining variation is insignificant, then this supports the normalization hypothesis.
Regarding the nature of social interactions on social media, particularly among mobilized users, and whether these interactions exhibit homophily (engagement with like-minded groups) or heterophily (engagement with a diverse range of opinions), (Fig. 1) the study proposes the following second hypothesis:
Combining the above theories and approaches, factors influencing online participation predominantly focus on the demographic characteristics of individuals (Boulianne, 2018), such as gender, age, education level, income level, regional affiliation, marital status, occupation, and religion (e.g., Anduiza et al., 2010; Best & Krueger, 2005; Dutton & Blank, 2011; Park, 2018; Saglie & Vabo, 2009; Shin et al., 2017; Smith, 2013; Vicente & Novo, 2014). Recent research has also directed attention to situational factors affecting online participation. Among these noteworthy research areas is privacy-related research (Hoffmann & Lutz, 2023; Lutz & Hoffmann, 2019). Privacy concerns have sparked debates on whether they prompt individuals to engage in more active online activities, including online participation, or conversely, deter them, turning them into passive consumers. This phenomenon is sometimes referred to as the ‘privacy paradox’ (Blank, 2013; Hoffmann & Lutz, 2023). In particular, where concerns about privacy invasion are high, there is a possibility of negative effects on online participation (Jang & Sung, 2021). Furthermore, overall privacy concerns associated with the online platform itself may lead individuals not only to refrain from participating but also to avoid using the platform altogether.
Variables, Measurement, and Data
This study utilizes data from the Korea Media Panel Survey (KMPS), which is published annually by the Korea Information Society Development Institute (KISDI).2 The dependent variable in this study is OPP. The first survey on OPP in the KMPS was conducted in 2012. Although there are concerns about the measurement of OPP due to its broad definition, a series of empirical studies (Jang, 2021; Oser et al., 2013; Park, 2018; Shin et al., 2017; Vicente & Novo, 2014) have used the frequency of key behaviors within OPP as dependent variables. These behaviors include ‘expressing opinions on public issues online (commenting, posting on forums, participating in discussions)’ and ‘participating in online voting, signing petitions, or opinion polls.’ Specific survey items3 include ‘frequency of writing original comments and replies on internet news/discussion boards over the past three months,’ ‘frequency of scrapping posts on internet news/discussion boards over the past three months,’ ‘frequency of online voting in the past three months,’ and ‘frequency of recommending and rating posts online over the past three months.’ The items are measured on a 6-point scale, ranging from ① rarely; ② about once every three months; ③ 1–3 times per month; ④ 1–3 times per week; ⑤ 4–6 times per week; to ⑥ almost every day. Following previous research using KMPS, we decided to use the average value of these four items.
The independent variable in this study is SMU. Despite concerns about measurement similar to those with OPP, the existing literature has predominantly used frequency variables to measure SMU (Hargittai & Hsieh, 2010; Jin & Yang, 2015; Kim & Ellison, 2022). Survey items for the independent variable include ‘frequency of writing posts and replies on social media in the past three months,’ ‘frequency of information sharing activities on social media in the past three months,’ and ‘frequency of recommending and rating posts on social media in the past three months.’ These are measured on a 6-point scale: ① almost never; ② about once every three months; ③ about 1–3 times per month; ④ about 1–3 times per week; ⑤ about 4–6 times per week; and ⑥ almost every day. We use the average value of these three items as the SMU variable. All other variables in the study are control variables, one of which is privacy concerns.
Table 1 presents the Cronbach’s Alpha values for various items related to OPP, SMU, and privacy concerns. These values indicate internal consistency reliability, which measures the homogeneity of the items within a scale. Typically, a value of 0.7 or higher is considered acceptable, 0.8 or higher is desirable, and values above 0.9 are viewed as excellent, despite some criticisms (see DeVellis, 2016; Taber, 2018). Each variable’s category demonstrates a degree of internal consistency that aligns with academic standards. Although detailed yearly results are omitted due to space constraints, it is noteworthy that these values have remained robust over time (OPP: 0.8367–0.8797, SMU: 0.934–0.950, privacy concerns: 0.955–0.985). In addition, income level, age group, educational background, religious affiliation, and employment status are also used to control for time-varying variations.
The instrumental variable used in this study is telecommunication costs (Zit): measured as the average monthly expense over the past three months, excluding the content bill, mobile payment bill, and monthly device installment. Concerning the use of telecommunication costs as an instrumental variable, it is crucial to consider their ability to control simultaneity when instrumental variables are derived from the same cross-sectional data (Angrist & Pischke, 2008). Instruments measured contemporaneously are limited in controlling for reverse causality. However, it is worth noting that the telecommunication costs variable results from a multi-year contract made in the past.
In South Korea, the structure of mobile device purchase agreements—often mandating a two-to-three-year contract for a subsidy on the device and communication costs—creates a scenario where telecommunication costs can act as a predeterminant variable suitable for controlling simultaneity in this study. Despite the enactment of the Mobile Device Distribution Improvement Act, aimed at regulating subsidy anomalies, fluctuations in these subsidies remain significant (Kim et al., 2017). Occasionally, these fluctuations allow consumers to acquire high-end devices like the latest iPhone for free. This randomness in subsidy amounts introduces variability in telecommunication costs, independent of factors that induce OPP, making telecommunication costs a viable instrument for analysis.
Analysis Methods: FE2SLS
Any effect of SMU might be the result of endogenous problems, especially reverse causality. For example, individuals who are active in OPP may also actively use social media. One way to address this endogeneity problem is to use actionable instrumental variables for SMU in empirical estimation. We employ the Fixed Effect Two-Stage Least Squares (FE2SLS) estimator (Schaffer, 2020), using the telecommunication costs variable as the instrumental variable. This approach is used to estimate the effect of SMU while controlling for unobserved factors and addressing reverse causality (Milner et al., 2018). The formula is as follows.
The first stage involves a fixed effect model where first-order difference operations are performed. In this model, SMUi represents the difference in individual i’s level of SMU between period t and period t+1, while Z(Phonebill)it represents the difference in telecommunication cost. Xit is the vector of control variables, and eit is the error term. The aim of the first stage is to isolate exogenous factors that cause changes in SMU, excluding unobserved confounders. Using the predicted value of SMU obtained from the first regression, we estimate the effect of SMU on OPP (Yit) in the second stage. Furthermore, to ensure the appropriate use of the instrumental variable, we examined its suitability by sequentially verifying the exogeneity assumption of the explanatory variables and the relevance of the instrumental variable, as is commonly done in such analyses (Wooldridge, 2010).
Empirical Results
Since this study employs a fixed-effects model that controls for individual time-invariant characteristics, our interpretation of the descriptive statistics focuses on the important time-varying variables (Table 2, 3, 4). Both OPP and SMU incrementally increased during the analysis period. However, the average of the OPP measurements is close to 1, and the distribution of OPP shows positive skewness. This suggests that most people do not engage in political activities online, and only a small number are highly active. Similarly, the sub-measurement variable of SMU, akin to that of OPP, indicates that SMU is not significantly high either. On the other hand, privacy concerns, a control variable, did not change significantly until 2017 but then rapidly increased by more than 1 point on average in that year, with an average of 3–4 points, indicating high levels of privacy concerns. Finally, telecommunication costs, the instrumental variable, is measured on a scale of 1 to 11 points. The average telecommunication costs did not change significantly during the analysis period, showing a slight overall decline.
However, when we decompose the variation, it reveals dynamics that were challenging to discern through simple trend analysis. Firstly, the within-variation for OPP is more significant than the between-variation. This suggests that there is more variability in OPP within the same individuals over time than across different individuals. Consequently, our analysis focuses on within-variation in OPP, which is why we test the mobilization hypothesis. Secondly, within-variations are larger than between-variations in SMU. This indicates that the variation is more influenced by changes within the same individuals over time. Thirdly, the between-variation in telecommunication cost is significantly higher than the within-variation. Among the time-varying variables, the telecommunication cost variable is the only one with higher between-variation than within-variation. This can be attributed to the randomness in subsidies. Lastly, within-variation is considerably higher than between-variation in the case of privacy concerns. This suggests that an individual’s level of concern about privacy is subject to more significant changes over time than the differences in privacy concerns among different individuals.
Using a fixed-effect model is preferable when there are not many observable variables available, and the risk of omitted variable bias is high. However, it is insufficient to handle all endogeneity issues, especially the problem of reverse causality (Schaffer, 2020; Wooldridge, 2010). If a reverse causal relationship exists between SMU and OPP, the effect of the independent variable can be underestimated when the instrumental variable is not used (e.g., Cawley & Meyerhoefer, 2012; Kwon, 2016). To address the remaining problem, we conduct FE2SLS (Model 1) alongside FE (Model 3), as well as RE2SLS (Model 2) and RE (Model 4) for efficient estimations additionally.
In the following, we examine whether the telecommunication costs variable is appropriate as the instrumental variable. Instrumental variables must meet the criteria of exogeneity, relevance, exclusivity, and identifiability. However, as the exogeneity of instrumental variables is challenging to identify statistically, the necessity of instrumental variables can only be confirmed by examining the endogeneity of explanatory variables (Schaffer, 2020; Wooldridge, 2010). The problem of endogeneity of explanatory variables relates to the correlation between the variation of the explanatory variables and the residual variation. The endogeneity of explanatory variables can be confirmed by calculating the F-statistic in multiple regression analysis, where the residual of the first-order regression equation is added as a variable to the second-order regression equation. In other words, the null hypothesis (Wooldridge, 2010). The analysis results reveal that the F-value, with degrees of freedom 1 and 9282, is 32.29, leading to the rejection of the null hypothesis that SMU has no endogeneity (p<0.0001)(Table 5).
Fixed effects estimation information | |
---|---|
Number of Groups=9,282 | Obs per group: min=2 |
Number of Obs=46,658 | Avg=5.3 |
Number of Clusters (pid)=9,282 | Max=6 |
Variable | Endogeniety | Underidentification | Weak IV |
---|---|---|---|
Social media use | F (1, 9282) | SW χ² (1) | SW F (1, 9282) |
32.29*** | 32.31*** | 32.29*** |
Next, we examine the relevance of the instrumental variable. Assessing the relevance of an instrumental variable involves determining whether there is a correlation between the instrumental variable and the explanatory variable that has been identified as endogenous. In other words, the null hypothesis is (Wooldridge, 2010). If the partial F-statistic value of the first-stage regression equation exceeds 10, then the instrumental variable is considered strong (Stock & Yogo, 2005). The analysis results reveal a t-value of 5.68 and a partial F-value of 32.31, indicating that the relevance condition is satisfied. Therefore, the instrumental variable in this study is not weak.4
Additionally, we test for underidentification of the simultaneous equations model. The analysis results show that the Kleibergen-Paap rk LM statistic, which follows a chi-square distribution with 1 degree of freedom, is 32.29. Consequently, the null hypothesis is rejected, confirming satisfaction of the ranking condition and indicating that there are no issues with the identification of the model. In summary, the instrumental variable is appropriately identified, and there are no concerns related to a weak instrumental variable. Therefore, we can confidently assert that the specification of the FE2SLS model is free from major concerns.
The analysis results indicate that there is a significant difference between the model using the instrumental variable and the model without the instrumental variable. Furthermore, the effect of SMU on OPP is underestimated in the models that exclude instrumental variables, consistent with previous findings. The Hausman test results show that the random effects model is not suitable because the null hypothesis is rejected in both cases, whether the instrumental variable is used or not. Therefore, we select Model 1 as the final model.
According to Model 1, a higher level of SMU is associated with a higher level of OPP. In other words, if the level of SMU increases by one unit between different time points within an individual, OPP increases by approximately 0.48. However, since this result is based on estimations using within variations, it cannot be interpreted as implying that individuals with high levels of SMU have higher OPP than those with low levels of SMU. This result suggests that the mobilization hypothesis is likely to be more acceptable than the normalization hypothesis and that the network features enough homophily to facilitate user participation (Table 6).
The result indicating that SMU has a significantly positive impact on OPP can be understood as an increase in opportunities for OPP resulting from SMU (Shirky, 2011). This is primarily because participation opportunities, such as online surveys, are often promoted on the same platform or across easily accessible platforms. Furthermore, the frequent and intensive interactions and exchanges of opinions among individuals through SMU may have inspired them to participate (Shirky, 2013).
Discussion
The ongoing debates regarding the utility of SMU are emblematic of broader discussions about whether technological advancements reflect existing societal states or catalyze change. This study contributes to this discourse by examining the effect of SMU in shaping OPP. Our finding aligns with prior research suggesting that SMU can act as a facilitator for civic engagement and participatory behaviors (Bennett & Segerberg, 2012; Theocharis & van Deth, 2017). This observation lends empirical support to the mobilization hypothesis and challenges the belief that technological advances in communication simply mirror existing participatory patterns (Norris, 2001).
Our finding warns of the risk of underestimation by unobserved time-varying factors. The effect size of SMU on OPP is significantly larger in the case of using the instrumental variable than not using it. This can be interpreted as the reverse causality problem severely exists (Kwon, 2016). Research supporting the normalization hypothesis in the context of SMU should be carefully interpreted. However, this study investigates the within variation, thereby, it is crucial to note that the normalization hypothesis is not entirely refuted. There is still a need to further explore the characteristics of groups that exhibit lower levels of OPP. Investigating which groups are underrepresented is vital for understanding the dynamics of representative democracy in the digital age (Park, 2018). The proportion of online participants is only 5%–6% in this dataset, even assuming an ideal sampling, it is difficult to argue that the online participants are highly representative of the entire population. Before considering whether it is necessary to promote SMU to facilitate OPP, thereby, it is necessary to continue the discussion on possible measures to promote the representativeness of the participants.
This study suggests that SMU can promote OPP, as seen in homophily networks, which is informed by social network theory. Contrary to concerns that high levels of heterophily can lead to political ambivalence and fear of isolation, resulting in decreased participation (Chu & Yeo, 2020), our findings indicate the opposite. Homophily networks can lead to the formation of echo chambers, where individuals are primarily exposed to information and opinions that reinforce their existing beliefs. This can exacerbate group thinking and confirmation bias, limiting exposure to diverse viewpoints and reducing critical thinking (Janis, 1972; Sunstein, 2001).
In recent years, social media has been identified as a major channel for propagating fake news (Torres et al., 2018). Some studies suggest that false or fake news spreads faster and more widely than genuine or credible news (e.g., Vosoughi et al., 2018). Unlike legacy media, social media does not have a fact-checking process, and most users share information without considering its authenticity and lack the ability to identify fake news. In the context of mobilization theory, misinformation, such as fake news and conspiracy theories, combined with confirmation bias in homophily networks, tends to become a more severe problem, deepening political polarization and increasing the transaction cost of society as a whole (Moravec et al., 2019). However, our finding indirectly represents the average effect of social network use. Therefore, further studies are needed to examine the mechanisms of political behavior in social networks (e.g., Bond & Sweitzer, 2022).
Conclusion
This study directly examines the controversy between mobilization and normalization theory, which has been somewhat overlooked in previous OPP studies. It aims to determine which theory has higher explanatory power and empirical support. Another merit of this study is its rigorous model selection process, which is based on initial analysis findings and strict methodological criteria. Furthermore, this study makes efforts to control various endogenous problems, including omitted variable bias and reverse causality. Lastly, it provides detailed methods for identifying an appropriate instrumental variable, an aspect that has often been overlooked in previous research, along with specific judgment criteria and test results for the instrumental variable’s use.
However, this study has some limitations as follows. First, there is a question regarding the validity of the instrument used. This study utilizes telecommunication costs at time ‘t’ rather than ‘t–1.’ While it can be argued that telecommunication costs from one or two years ago, determined by multi-year contracts and past plans, have minimal impact on current SMU, there remains an inherent issue that FE2SLS may not be a valid tool for controlling endogeneity, despite these rationales. Additionally, there is a fundamental limitation in making causal claims using observational data, raising methodological concerns. Second, there is a concern about whether the measurement of online participation is appropriate. According to the literature (e.g., Hrastinski, 2009; Kim & Hoewe, 2020; Norris, 2002), online participation is a broad concept encompassing various behaviors unrelated to elections or voting, such as discussions, article writing, and replies to articles. While this study measures OPP with multiple items consistent with previous research and demonstrates internal validity, the measurement may lack intuitive clarity and could have potential content validity issues to some extent. Given the constraints of using secondary data, these limitations may be challenging to overcome. Future research should address these limitations through more rigorous measurements and experimental designs.